- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Socioeconomic status, linguistic skills and language background differentially relate to preschoolers’ emotional and behavioural profile
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health volume 18, Article number: 133 (2024)
Abstract
Background
Proximal and distal factors interact to shape children’s development and well-being. The present study aimed to examine socioeconomic status (SES), linguistic skills, and language background as concurrent predictors of socio-emotional and behavioural outcomes in heritage bilingual and monolingual children attending preschool.
Methods
Parents of 1810 children (mean age = 63.42 months ± 7.36), attending preschool in Italy, completed the Four Factor Index of SES and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Teachers (n = 99) completed a questionnaire on children’s linguistic skills and emotional and behavioural profiles. A subsample of 995 children was administered an expressive vocabulary task in Italian.
Results
Regression analyses showed that linguistic skills were the only concurrent predictor of conduct problems, as well as the dominant predictor of hyperactivity/impulsivity, peer problems, and better prosocial behaviour. SES was negatively related to ADHD traits, peer problems, and prosocial behaviour. Finally, heritage bilingualism background was associated, although not as a primary predictor, with increased emotional problems, peer relationship problems, and lower teacher-rated emotional and behavioural skills. However, it was the main factor positively associated with prosocial behaviour.
Conclusions
The implications of these findings for research in this area and for educational policy are discussed, highlighting the need for a multidimensional perspective that includes linguistic skills and SES in the assessment of children’s emotional and behavioural outcomes.
Background
Multidimensional models clearly suggest that environmental (distal) factors interact with individual (proximal) factors in shaping children’s developmental trajectories [1, 2]; linguistic skills and socioeconomic status (SES) have been widely examined in the literature as potential protective (or risk) factors for children’s emotional and behavioural outcomes. Linguistic skills have been found to be related to emotional and behavioural development in typical children [3] and atypical (i.e., with developmental language disorders) populations [4, 5]. More in-depth studies have then explored these trends even in bilingual populations (i.e., children from families with a migratory background) [6]. SES has been found to underlie children’s discrepancies in academic achievements [7,8,9,10], linguistic skills [11,12,13], and socio-emotional well-being [14, 15]. However, SES is better viewed as a structural variable rather than an intrinsic characteristic of the family/individual itself [16] and the impact of SES may be modulated by other social and family characteristics [17, 18]. In addition, some meta-analyses [19] found diminished, although significant, effect sizes of the relationship between SES and academic achievement than suggested by previous studies [20], with the effect size being more robust in more economically developed countries [21] and in younger children compared to older ones [22]. Finally, the definition and operationalization of SES is still a matter of debate and results might depend on how it is conceptualized and measured [23].
Following globalisation and migration routes, many children attending school in Western countries live in families with a migratory background and are often exposed to one or more heritage languages within the family. For the purpose of the present study, the term “heritage bilinguals” (HB) will be adopted in line with current literature [24], although plenty of different labels are available in previous studies [25]. In preschool years, HB may have lower second language (L2) proficiency, compared to monolingual peers, given to the still limited exposure to the societal language [24], and they might experience socio-economic disadvantage. In Italy, where the present study has been carried out, the index of absolute poverty is 35.6% for families with both parents holding non-Italian citizenship compared to 6.4% of families with Italian citizenship [26]. The present study focuses on understanding the differential contribution of SES and language proficiency as concurrent predictors of children’s socio-emotional and behavioural outcomes in a broad sample that includes Italian monolingual children and HB, thus taking into account family language background as an additional distal factor.
The relationships between language skills and socio-emotional well-being
Although language and emotional/behavioural difficulties often co-occur, the direction of the relationship is harder to define [27]. Some authors suggest a bidirectional relationship with reciprocal influences between the two [28], whereas others support unidirectional pathways as being more likely, with language difficulties as a prominent factor leading to socio-emotional and behavioural difficulties [29, 30]. In this line, [31] reported that the main predictor of children’s well-being was vocabulary growth from 4 to 9 years rather than vocabulary knowledge at four years. A study by Bichay-Awadalla et al. [32] on preschoolers reported, instead, different patterns of relationships with socio-emotional development according to different aspects of linguistic skills, with bidirectional patterns for expressive skills and unidirectional patterns for receptive skills. Other perspectives suggest the possibility that the relationship between linguistic skills and emotional and behavioural problems might be mediated by other (third) factors or that behavioural and emotional problems might hinder full language development [27].
Some studies have also explored these trends in HB children. Specifically, the literature has found associations between problematic behaviours and lower linguistic competence in preschool bilingual children [6, 33,34,35], with greater externalising behaviours in primary school children with lower L2 linguistic skills [36, 37].
There may be adverse factors related to the migratory process of some families, such as interrupted education, poverty, frequent transitions, exposure to violence, and separation from family, that might increase vulnerability [38,39,40]. Therefore, language background shouldn’t be considered only in terms of language competence but as a more complex variable that includes the potential impact on the child’s well-being of speaking a language at home that’s different from the one employed in social contexts. The perception that the heritage language is devalued or under-recognised as a linguistic ability both at school and in social contexts could affect the family climate and, consequently, the child’s well-being. In line with this, some authors reported an increased risk for emotional and behavioural problems in HB children and adolescents [41], although some studies did not report differences in prosocial behavior and social skills between HB and monolingual peers [42]. On the other hand, positive attitudes towards multilingualism and good connections with heritage language and culture could foster enriched social inclusion, emotional well-being and family cohesion [9, 43,44,45,46,47].
The relationships between socioeconomic status and children’s verbal, emotional and behavioural skills
The impact of SES on language and socio-emotional outcomes could be explained by multiple mechanisms. On the one side, families from low socioeconomic backgrounds may have less access to materials and information, social resources and educational opportunities (e.g., exposure to books, reading practice, quality of schools, etc.) to support children's verbal and emotional development [48]. In addition, the availability of material resources may affect subjective experience and how individuals shape their experiences and behaviors (e.g., fewer resources may be associated with an increased focus on external social forces and other individuals that influence one's life outcomes [49]. Some data from the literature suggest that SES has a direct impact on children’s linguistic skills: for example, vocabulary knowledge and vocabulary growth, syntax and language learning process [12, 13, 50], as well as literacy skills [51, 52] are known to be negatively affected by lower levels of SES. However, some authors [8], also evidenced that some additional factors (i.e., higher country SES and alphabetic languages) might induce a larger indirect impact from SES to reading ability. Also, further evidence suggests that other variables such as parental participation and school resources [53] may buffer the SES-academic achievement relationship. A large and recent study [54] found that SES (operationalized as maternal education) and multilingualism were not significantly associated with children’s verbal production over the first 4 years of life, but other variables related to the home environment, such as the amount of adult talk, were predictors of children’s linguistic skills. It should be considered that previous studies have found high variability within both low- and high-SES families in home-related variables such as their home support for learning (i.e., home literacy/numeracy) [7, 55, [56]. Evidence has been collected on the relationship between SES and verbal abilities in HB populations. SES has been found to be related to HBs’ L2 vocabulary size [57, 58], productive and receptive morphosyntactic skills [59, 60] and discursive-semantic abilities [61] in the latter study, it was also observed that the greater the cumulative exposure to school language, the stronger the correlation between SES and sentence repetition score.
SES might also have an impact on children’s socio-emotional and behavioural development [62] and has been found to be linked to the development of psychopathology and lower academic achievement [10, 63,64,65]. In a meta-analysis [66], it was found that SES was more strongly related to externalising rather than internalising symptoms, but it emerged that the relationship between SES and child psychopathology is likely to vary in different populations of children and in different communities. Furthermore, some authors reported a weak association between SES and psychopathological vulnerability in some ethnic groups [67, 68]. The association between SES and psychopathological indicators, therefore, is still a matter of debate, particularly for HB children.
Present study
Based on previous literature and theoretical multidimensional models, multiple relationships emerge among SES, linguistic development, language background, and emotional and behavioural development. However, there is a need for more extensive research that accounts for the influence of both proximal and distal factors on children’s outcomes, particularly for the emotional and behavioural dimensions. As evidenced by Singh and Rajendra [69] greater attention to socioeconomic status in developmental research is still needed to increase the generalizability of data in psychological research. This is particularly the case when studying specific populations, as there is a risk of biased inferences if both proximal and distal factors are not accounted for [24]. Finally, most of the previous work has been conducted in English-speaking contexts, and there might be cultural differences that limit the generalisability of results from one context to another. It is therefore necessary to develop studies in different cultural contexts, and the present study, conducted in Italy, may represent an important contribution to the previous literature.
The aim of the present study is to investigate, with a multi-informant (parents, teachers) and multi-method (questionnaires, tests) approach, differences and similarities in linguistic and socioemotional/behavioural skills between HB and monolingual peers attending preschool, as well as the role of SES, linguistic skills and language background as concurrent predictors of socio-emotional and behavioural skills in HB children and monolingual peers.
The main research question of the present study is: what is the association of linguistic background, SES and linguistic skills on socioemotional and behavioural skills in HB and monolingual peers?
Given that previous literature highlighted influences of linguistic background, linguistic skills and SES on socioemotional and behavioural skills, we wanted to analyse the differential role of linguistic background, SES and linguistic skills, considered together, on children’s socioemotional and behavioural skills. We expect SES [10, 62,63,64,65,66] and linguistic skills [29, 30, 32] to be the main predictors of children’s well-being, and once accounted for these variables, to observe a marginal role of HB per se.
A preliminary analysis of group differences between HB and monolingual peers was also conducted to: (a) report what the analysis of group differences between HB children and monolingual peers in socio-emotional and behavioral skills might suggest when SES and language skills are not taken into account. This type of comparison runs the risk of overestimating differences based on language background alone; (b) to justify the inclusion of the dichotomous (HB vs. monolinguals) group variable in the regression models addressing the main research question.
Method
Participants
The study involved a community sample of 1810 children attending the second and third year of 58 public all-day preschools, located in Bologna, a city in northern Italy. Participants were recruited as part of a larger project aimed at enhancing language skills in preschoolers in collaboration with the Municipality of Bologna; a letter was sent to parents with informed consent and questionnaires, participation was voluntary. Children whose parents provided consent were administered the vocabulary task and data from the teacher questionnaire were entered. The teachers completed the questionnaire on individual children as an action of the larger project. Within this sample, 1261 children were from families speaking mainly Italian in the family context (monolinguals), and 549 were HB children. The children were classified as HB if the parents reported in the questionnaire that they spoke a Heritage Language in the family context. Further, most families had a migrant background, with one parent (29.3%) or both (59.4%) born in another country, whereas 11.3% had both parents speaking a heritage language in the family but were born in Italy. For more extended demographic details see Table 1. Socioeconomic status was assessed through the Hollingshead Four Factor Index of Social Status (see Materials section).
A few children (n = 19) had a certified mild disability. All parents filled in the study questionnaire (see below for details). Then, for each child, a teacher filled in a questionnaire, for a total of 99 teachers involved. Teachers filled in the questionnaire for a variable number of children depending on how many families adhered to the study in each class. The Vocabulary task (see below for details) was administered to a subsample of 995 children, of which 31.76% are in the HB group. The Italian preschool system involves children aged from 3 to 6 years old and is structured as follows: a three-year program during which no formal teaching of literacy is provided. However, the children might be engaged in activities to improve their language skills in playful activities.
Materials
Questionnaires for parents
Socio-economic status (SES)
Parents completed the Hollingshead Four Factor Index of Social Status. To achieve a composite score for each child’s SES, information regarding both parents’ educational level and occupation was scored from 1 to 7 for educational level and 1 to 9 for occupation. Then, SES scores for each parent were calculated using the formula (educational level*3 + occupation*5); the mean between parents’ SES was used as the child’s SES. In the case of single parents, their unique score was used. The minimum and maximum scores ranged from 8 to 66. Scores between 8 and 29 are considered low-medium SES, scores between 30 and 30 are defined and medium SES and scores above 40 are considered medium-high SES.
Strengths and difficulties questionnaire
The single-sided version of the SDQ-parents [70] was administered. The SDQ is available in many languages and parents were offered the possibility to access their preferred language when requested. The questionnaire has been found to be concordant for both native and immigrant parents [71]. This questionnaire includes 25 items describing positive and negative behavioural traits; respondents use a 3-point Likert-type scale (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat true, and 2 = certainly true) to rate each item referring to the son/daughter. The 25 items are divided among the following five scales (Cronbach alpha reliability scores are calculated on the study sample): Emotional Symptoms (α = 0.66), Conduct Problems (α = 0.53), Hyperactivity-Inattention (α = 0.70), Peer Relationship Problems (α = 0.60), Prosocial Behaviour (α = 0.65). A higher score corresponds to more severe difficulties on the four scales describing negative behaviours. On the Prosocial Behaviour scale, a higher score indicates more positive behaviours. For each scale, the maximum score is 10. The observed reliabilities show poor to acceptable values [72], in line with previous studies investigating psychometric properties of the SDQ-parents in Italian samples of older students [73, 74].
Questionnaire for teachers
Children’s early linguistic skills (in Italian) and emotional-behavioural profile were assessed with a proxy-report questionnaire administered to their teachers. The whole questionnaire consists of 20 items [18, 75, 76], developed based on the early cognitive, literacy, numeracy, and behavioural skills deemed adequate for preschoolers based on the Italian curriculum and the previous literature. The questions were first qualitatively validated by groups of teachers who provided feedback on the items’ clarity. For each item, the name of the competence was accompanied by a short definition and some examples (e.g., phonological awareness: “It refers to the child’s ability to perform fusion/segmentation tasks, such as splitting or joining the pieces of the word banana: ba-na-na). For the aim of the present study, we considered only two subscales:
-
Linguistic skills (Five items: phonological awareness, morphosyntactic comprehension and production, narrative skills, pre-writing skills);
-
Behavioural profile (Four items: the ability to respect waiting time, sociality, emotional resources, interest in activities).
The teachers rated their evaluations of the children’s skills on a five-point Likert scale from “never/absent” to “always/excellent competence”. The Cronbach’s alpha for the scales is 0.904 for the linguistic area and 0.968 for the behavioural area.
Children’s expressive vocabulary
The early language skills of the children were assessed through the expressive vocabulary task, administered in Italian, from the Learning Difficulties Indexes– IDA [18]. In this task, children were asked to name 36 images disposed on three grids, with 12 images each selected for decreasing frequency in spoken language [77]. The accuracy score, ranging from 0 to 36 (1 point for each correct answer), was considered. The Cronbach’s alpha of the scale was 0.85, according to the test manual.
Procedure
Questionnaires on SES and SDQ were provided to parents as paper and pencil questionnaires. Parents could complete it together or by who spends more time with the child, usually the mother. The teachers were required to complete the questionnaire for each child within one month to allow them to observe the children’s linguistic skills and emotional and behavioural skills. Early language tasks in Italian were administered individually by a trained psychologist in a quiet room at the children’s school in a single session lasting about 10 min. Special attention was given to ascertaining children had correctly understood the instructions. Participants involved in the study gave informed consent, the study was conducted in accordance in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the University of Bologna Bioethical Committee approved the project (Prot. 322431, December 21, 2021).
Data analysis
We ran the analysis using the program RStudio 1.0.153 [78]. We used the “lmtest” [79] and “car” [80] for our main analyses. As preliminary analyses, Pearson correlation analyses among the main variables of the study are reported, with the aim of examining the relationships between the key variables in our study. For this purpose, we used the “hmisc” package [81]. Then, group differences (monolinguals vs. HB) across the main measures considered were verified through Welch Two Sample t-tests, which are particularly robust when there is high variability within a sample (i.e., both for socioeconomic and linguistic background).
The second set of models aimed to understand how language background (dichotomic variable), together with SES and verbal knowledge (continuous variables), related respectively to parents’ and teachers’ evaluation of children’s socioemotional and behavioural skills (measured through SDQ and ad-hoc teacher questionnaire respectively). Given the high correlation between the vocabulary task and the assessment of linguistic skills through teachers’ questionnaire (r =.550), we decided to include in the analyses only the latter variable to avoid collinearity and consider a wider sample, therefore reported results are referred to the sample size of 1810 participants. Furthermore, we ran dominance analyses for every model. Indeed, this analysis has been used to determine the relative importance of every predictor inserted in our regressions. The “relaimpo” package [82] has been employed for this purpose.
Results
Preliminary analysis: correlations and group differences
Correlations among the main variables of the study are reported in Table 2, group differences are reported in Table 3.
As it can be observed from Table 3, HB belonged to an averagely lower socioeconomic background compared to monolinguals and performed significantly worse in the vocabulary task, in linguistic skills, and in the behavioural profile according to the teacher’s evaluation. Concerning SDQ Scales, except for the behavioural scale, bilingual children had significantly higher rates of reported difficulties in all domains, as reported their parents’ answers to the questionnaire. However, they also had higher values on the Prosocial Skills Scale, meaning that, according to their parents, they tend to be more prosocial in comparison with their monolingual peers. Since these comparisons did not take into account the role of SES and L2 linguistic skills further models were developed to better investigate if these differences based on language background hold when other variables are taken into account.
Concurrent predictors of socio-emotional and behavioural profile
All results of the regression analyses are presented in Table 4. For each analysis, language background, linguistic skills and SES were considered predictors and, in turn, SDQ subscales and teacher ratings were considered as dependent variables.
Emotional symptoms
The categorical variable Language Background was significant (p <.05, β = 0.240), meaning that bilinguals have averagely higher scores of emotional symptoms according to parents’ reports. The continuous variable L2 linguistic skills was not significant (p =.188), while SES was significant (p <.05, β = − 0.008), meaning that children coming from higher socioeconomic status families show overall less emotional problems. The dominance analysis reveals that SES’ contribution seems to be particularly relevant in the present model.
Conduct problems
The categorial variable Language Background was not significant (p =.127).
The continuous variable linguistic skills was significant, with p <.001, with a negative estimate β = -0.144, meaning that children with higher performance in the verbal domain show fewer conduct problems. On the contrary, SES was not significant, with a p =.093. The dominance analysis reveals that linguistic skills contribution seems to be particularly relevant in the present model.
Hyperactivity/Inattention scale
The categorial variable Language Background was not significant (p =.375). The continuous variables L2 linguistic skills and SES were both significant, with p <.001, β = -0.374 and β = -0.020, meaning that children with higher performance in the verbal domain in Italian show fewer hyperactivity/inattention problems, as well as children coming from higher SES families. The dominance analysis reveals that the contribution of L2 linguistic skills emerges as particularly significant in the current model, demonstrating a stronger and more influential association compared to the other variables examined.
Peer relationship problems
The categorial variable Language Background was significant (p <.001) with a positive estimate (β = 0.485). This result indicates that HBs have average higher peer relationship problems compared to monolinguals. The continuous variables L2 linguistic skills and SES were both significant with p <.001, β = − 0.265 and β = − 0.014, meaning that children with an averagely better performance in L2 linguistic skills, as well as children coming from higher socioeconomic status families, show overall fewer peer relationships problems. The dominance analysis indicates that L2 linguistic skills appear to be notably significant within the present model, suggesting a pronounced influence compared to other variables under investigation.
Prosocial behaviour scale
The categorial variable language background was significant (p <.001) with a positive estimate (β = 0.383). This result means that bilinguals have an average higher prosocial behaviour. The continuous L2 linguistic skills and SES were both significant with a positive estimate for linguistic skills (p <.001, β = 0.212) and a negative estimate for SES (p <.01, β = − 0.011), meaning that children with better L2 linguistic skills have better prosocial behaviour, but those coming from higher socioeconomic status families show lower prosocial behaviour skills. The dominance analysis underscores the notable significance of bilingualism’s contribution within the current model, indicating a discernible role that surpasses that of other variables under examination.
Teachers’ evaluation on children’s socioemotional and behavioural skills
Concerning the model on teachers’ evaluation of children’s socioemotional and behavioural skills, language background turned out to be significant, with p <.001 and β = -0.183, meaning that HBs received by teachers lower scores in socioemotional and behavioural skills. L2 Linguistic skills and SES also turned out to be significant, respectively with p <.001, β = 0.653 and p <.01, β = -0.004, indicating that children with higher linguistic skills are evaluated as significantly better in terms of socioemotional and behavioural skills. On the contrary, a higher SES was associated with lower scores in terms of socioemotional and behavioural skills. The dominance analysis highlights the substantial relevance of linguistic skills within the current model, indicating a pronounced influence that exceeds that of other variables under investigation.
Discussion
The present study investigated the differential contribution of language background (monolinguals vs. HB), SES and linguistic skills on children’s emotional and behavioural profiles.
A preliminary analysis of group differences between HB children and monolingual peers was conducted in order to justify the inclusion of a dichotomous variable (language background) in the regression models and to highlight the risk of overestimating differences based on language background alone if other variables such as SES and linguistic skills are not considered.
Based on dichotomic comparison, a picture of increased emotional problems, hyperactivity/impulsivity traits and peer relationship difficulties emerged in HB children, compared to the monolingual group. There were no differences in conduct problems and an advantage in prosocial behaviour in HBs, based on parents’ reports. Also, this group received lower scores in the evaluation of emotional and behavioural skills, according to teachers’ reports. Together, in line with previous studies [24, 83, 84], HBs had lower SES and linguistic skills (in the majority language, i.e., Italian), the latter measured either through direct assessment (vocabulary task) or teachers’ evaluations, compared to children from monolingual backgrounds. This first set of results might suggest, in line with previous studies, that HB children might be at higher risk of emotional and behavioural difficulties [41].
However, this representation might oversimplify a complex phenomenon, and the study aimed to delve deeper into this initial picture and add new evidence through an analysis of the differential relationships of language background, together with composite measures of SES and linguistic skills evaluation with emotional and behavioural profile. The five main dimensions of the SDQ questionnaire (parents’ reports) were considered independently, together with teachers’ evaluations of children’s emotional and behavioural skills.
To sum up, moving from dichotomic group analyses to regression models that consider and control the differential role of language background, SES and (Italian) linguistic skills, a more complex pattern of relationships emerged. Linguistic skills were actually found to be the unique concurrent predictor for conduct problems and the dominant predictor for the hyperactivity/impulsivity traits, the problems in peer relationships and the emotional and behavioural skills evaluated by teachers. Also, linguistic skills positively predicted prosocial behaviour. However, linguistic skills were not significantly related to emotional problems as assessed by parents, which had SES as their main predictor instead, followed by language background. There was also a negative significant relationship between lower SES and hyperactivity/impulsivity traits, problems in peer relationships, and prosocial behaviour. Higher SES also predicted better emotional and behavioural skills according to teacher evaluation. Finally, HB background was associated, but not as a primary predictor, with increased emotional problems and peer relationship problems as assessed by parents, and minor emotional and behavioural skills as assessed by teachers. However, it was the main factor to be positively associated with prosocial behaviour.
This picture can be interpreted in light of previous literature, suggesting a main association of L2 linguistic skills with children’s behavioural profiles, with particular emphasis on externalising traits and social relationships [3, 6, 29, 30, 32]. This study was conducted on a typical population, suggesting that the children who have lower L2 linguistic skills might encounter more difficulties in peer relationships and might show increased behavioural problems, possibly due to minor access to verbal communication. SES was found to be primarily associated with emotional problems, in line with previous literature [10, 63,64,65], suggesting that resources might shape subjective experiences. Differently from what is stated by Peverill et al. [66], findings from the present study seem to suggest a main association with internalising rather than externalising symptoms. Also, SES generally resulted as a non-dominant factor compared to linguistic skills in predicting patterns of socioemotional and behavioural difficulties, suggesting that proximal factors might be prominent over distal factors. This sheds light on the idea that the influence of SES should not be viewed as deterministic or inherent to individuals [16], but rather as a structural variable or a constraint, according to the neuroconstructivist perspective [2], which individuals and society can intervene on. In this regard, previous literature found that the family environment can mediate the influence of SES on children’s early achievements and well-being [18, 85].
Finally, as hypothesised, the strength of the relationship between language background and socioemotional and behavioural problems was less conspicuous, although still present, when L2 linguistic skills and SES were taken into account. As outlined by Araújo Dawson and Williams [36], language status can act as an acculturative stressor throughout the early school experience and can negatively affect well-being. However, an innovative result is increased prosocial behaviours in HB children, whilst lower in high SES children. So, even if HB children might experience a degree of increased difficulty in engaging in peer relationships and might be more at risk for emotional problems, they seem to have active coping strategies that bring them to engage in prosocial behaviours. As previous literature suggests, bilingualism might be associated with both cognitive and everyday life advantages [86, 87].
The study presents some limitations that need to be acknowledged. First of all, results are based on a concurrent design, and this does not allow the definition of causal patterns. As previous evidence suggested [27], bidirectional relationships are also possible, at least in terms of linguistic skills and emotional patterns. It is, instead, more plausible to assume unidirectional patterns of SES and HB to emotional well-being. Secondly, teachers’ evaluation might also be biased by a halo effect that might lead to high correlations between linguistic and behavioural judgments. Also, the teacher’s prejudices against low-SES children could influence their competence assessment. However, correlation analyses highlighted that vocabulary skills were more strongly related to the teachers’ evaluation of linguistic skills than to behavioural items. It has also to be underlined that in the present study we only evaluated linguistic skills in the majority language (Italian), whereas future studies might also include assessment on heritage language. Another point regards the measurement and operationalization of SES. In the present study education and occupation levels from both parents were considered as a composite measure, but other approaches might be adopted [23]. Finally, the present study did not take into account important sources of heterogeneity in the HB sample (e.g., linguistic input and age of exposure to heritage language and L2) [24], and future investigation should also consider positive and negative attitudes toward heritage language in the child and the family [43,44,45,46,47] in order to better investigate under which circumstances HB might be at greater risk of emotional discomfort.
Conclusions
Despite its limitations, the present study highlights some innovative patterns that might represent new evidence of the differential relationship between SES, linguistic skills and language background on children’s socioemotional and behavioural profiles and might lead to potential implications for research and practice. Linguistic skills (in the majority language, i.e., Italian) and, secondly, SES turned out to be the dominant factors associated with socio-emotional and behavioural well-being. Heritage bilinguals might be at higher risk of emotional discomfort but they also show some advantages in prosocial behaviour, unlike children with higher SES. Finally, the study has been conducted in Italy, where minor evidence has been collected so far on the relationship between the above-cited variables and therefore represents an original contribution to previous literature.
As regards the potential implications, also in line with Singh and Rajendra [69], these results underline the importance of including composite measures of SES in psychological research and suggest that both linguistic skills and SES should be included when investigating children’s socioemotional and behavioural well-being, going beyond categorical group comparisons. In terms of practice, it further reinforces the significance of early interventions on linguistic skills in preschool settings. Further, the study highlights the importance of encouraging a multidimensional approach among educators and clinicians when faced with children with emotional and behavioural difficulties, specifically in the case of HB, developing multiple hypotheses on the factors underlying the discomfort and favouring multi-component interventions.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in PsychArchives at https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.23668/psycharchives.14488.
References
Bronfenbrenner U, Morris PA. The bioecological model of human development. Handbook Child Psychol. 2007. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0114.
Westermann G, Mareschal D, Johnson MH, Sirois S, Spratling MW, Thomas MSC. Neuroconstructivism. Dev Sci. 2007;10(1):75–83. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00567.x.
Chow JC, Wehby JH. Associations between language and problem behaviour: a systematic review and correlational meta-analysis. Educ Psychol Rev. 2018;30(1):61–82. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10648-016-9385-z.
Curtis PR, Frey JR, Watson CD, Hampton LH, Roberts MY. Language disorders and problem behaviours: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2018. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1542/peds.2017-3551.
Yew SGK, O’Kearney R. Emotional and behavioural outcomes later in childhood and adolescence for children with specific language impairments: meta-analyses of controlled prospective studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54(5):516–24. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/jcpp.12009.
Sun H, Yussof NTB, Mohamed MBBH, Rahim AB, Bull R, Cheung MWL, Cheong SA. Bilingual language experience and children’s social-emotional and behavioural skills: a cross-sectional study of Singapore preschoolers. Int J Bilingual Educ Biling. 2021;24(3):324–39. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/13670050.2018.1461802.
Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Ann Rev Psychol. 2002;53:371–99. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233.
Li J, Peng P, Ma XE, Ding N, Zhao J. How does family socioeconomic status influence children’s reading ability? Evidence from meta-analytic structural equation modeling. Educ Psychol Rev. 2023;35(4):119. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.31234/osf.io/7yv6r.
Mueller LM, Howard K, Wilson E, Gibson J, Katsos N. Bilingualism in the family and child well-being: a scoping review. Int J Biling. 2020;24(5–6):1049–70. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/1367006920920939.
Sirin SR. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: a meta-analytic review of research. Rev Educ Res. 2005;75(3):417–53. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3102/00346543075003417.
Gibson JL, Newbury DF, Durkin K, Pickles A, Conti-Ramsden G, Toseeb U. Pathways from the early language and communication environment to literacy outcomes at the end of primary school; the roles of language development and social development. Oxf Rev Educ. 2021;47(2):260–83. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/03054985.2020.1824902.
Hoff E. The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev. 2003;74(5):1368–78. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/1467-8624.00612.
Hoff E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev Rev. 2006;26(1):55–88. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002.
Joshi H, Cooksey E, Wiggins R, McCulloch A, Verropoulou G, Clarke L. Diverse family living situations and child development: a multi-level analysis comparing longitudinal evidence from Britain and the United States. Int J Law Policy Family. 1999;13(3):292–314. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/lawfam/13.3.292.
Poulain T, Vogel M, Kiess W. Review on the role of socioeconomic status in child health and development. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2020;32(2):308–14. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1097/mop.0000000000000876.
Hannon P, Nutbrown C, Morgan A. Effects of extending disadvantaged families’ teaching of emergent literacy. Res Papers Educ. 2020;35(3):310–36. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/02671522.2019.1568531.
Benzies K, Mychasiuk R. Fostering family resiliency: a review of the key protective factors. Child Family Social Work. 2009;14(1):103–14. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1365-2206.2008.00586.x.
Bonifacci P, Giuliano P, Pellizzari C, Serra P. IDA - Indicatori delle Difficoltà Di Apprendimento. Hogrefe; 2015.
Harwell M, Maeda Y, Bishop K, Xie A. The surprisingly modest relationship between SES and educational achievement. J Exp Educ. 2017;85(2):197–214. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/00220973.2015.1123668.
Coleman JS. Equality of educational opportunity. Equity Excell Educ. 1968;6(5):19–28. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/0020486680060504.
Kim Sw, Cho H, Kim LY. Socioeconomic status and academic outcomes in developing countries: a meta-analysis. Rev Educ Res. 2019;89(6):875–916. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3102/0034654319877155.
Strohschein L. Household income histories and child mental health trajectories. J Health Soc Behav. 2005;46(4):359–75. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/002214650504600404.
Antonoplis S. Studying socioeconomic status: conceptual problems and an alternative path forward. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2022;18(2):275–92. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/17456916221093615.
Paradis J. Sources of individual differences in the dual language development of heritage bilinguals. J Child Lang. 2023;50(4):793–817. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0305000922000708.
Surrain S, Luk G. Describing bilinguals: a systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Biling Lang Cogn. 2019;22(2):401–15. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S1366728917000682.
ISTAT. (2024). Povertà assoluta e spese per consumi. Retrieved by: https://www.istat.it/it/files/2024/03/STAT_TODAY_POVERTA-ASSOLUTA_2023_25.03.24.pdf
Hinshaw SP. Externalizing behaviour problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychol Bull. 1992;111(1):127–55. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.127.
Girard L-C, Pingault J-B, Doyle O, Falissard B, Tremblay RE. Developmental associations between conduct problems and expressive language in early childhood: a population-based study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44(6):1033–43. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10802-015-0094-8.
Bornstein MH, Hahn C-S, Suwalsky JTD. Language and internalizing and externalizing behavioural adjustment: developmental pathways from childhood to adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(3):857–78. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0954579413000217.
Petersen IT, Bates JE, D’Onofrio BM, Coyne CA, Lansford JE, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Van Hulle CA. Language ability predicts the development of behaviour problems in children. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122(2):542–57. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/a0031963.
Westrupp EM, Reilly S, McKean C, Law J, Mensah F, Nicholson JM. Vocabulary development and trajectories of behavioural and emotional difficulties via academic ability and peer problems. Child Dev. 2020;91(2):e365-82. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/cdev.13219.
Bichay-Awadalla K, Qi CH, Bulotsky-Shearer RJ, Carta JJ. Bidirectional relationship between language skills and behaviour problems in preschool children from low-income families. J Emot Behav Disord. 2020;28(2):114–28. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/1063426619853535.
Han W-J. Bilingualism and socioemotional well-being. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2010;32(5):720–31. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.01.009.
Sun H. Home environment, bilingual preschooler’s receptive mother tongue language outcomes, and social-emotional and behavioural skills: one stone for two birds? Front Psychol. 2019. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01640.
Winsler A, Kim YK, Richard ER. Socio-emotional skills, behaviour problems, and Spanish competence predict the acquisition of English among English language learners in poverty. Dev Psychol. 2014;50(9):2242–54. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/a0037161.
Araújo Dawson B, Williams SA. The impact of language status as an acculturative stressor on internalizing and externalizing behaviours among latino/a children: a longitudinal analysis from school entry through third grade. J Youth Adolesc. 2008;37(4):399–411. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10964-007-9233-z.
McNally S, Darmody M, Quigley J. The socio-emotional development of language-minority children entering primary school in Ireland. Ir Educational Stud. 2019;38(4):519–34. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/03323315.2019.1663550.
Fazel M, Stein A. The mental health of refugee children. Archives of disease in childhood. 2002;87(5):366-70. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1136/adc.87.5.366.
Hadfield K, Ostrowski A, Ungar M. What can we expect of the mental health and well-being of Syrian refugee children and adolescents in Canada? Canadian Psychology / Psychologie Canadienne. 2017;58(2):194–201. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/cap0000102.
Sirin SR, Rogers-Sirin L. The educational and mental health needs of Syrian refugee children. Washington, DC: Migration Policy Institute. 2015;13.
Belhadj Kouider E, Koglin U, Petermann F. Emotional and behavioural problems in migrant children and adolescents in Europe: a systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;23(6):373–91. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s00787-013-0485-8.
Jurkic A, Halliday SE, Hascher T. The relationship of language and social competence of preschool- and kindergarten-age single and dual language learners in Switzerland and Germany. Early Child Res Q. 2023;64:72–83. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.ecresq.2023.02.003.
Cangelosi M, Borghetti C, Bonifacci P. How parents’ perceived value of the heritage language predicts their children’s skills. Languages. 2024;9(3):80.
De Houwer A. Harmonious bilingual development: young families’ well-being in language contact situations. Int J Biling. 2015;19(2):169–84. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/1367006913489202.
Halle TG, Whittaker JV, Zepeda M, Rothenberg L, Anderson R, Daneri P, Wessel J, Buysse V. The social–emotional development of dual language learners: looking back at existing research and moving forward with purpose. Early Child Res Q. 2014;29(4):734–49. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.12.002.
Hollebeke I. How stable is a family’s language policy? Multilingual families’ beliefs, practices, and management across time. Lang Policy. 2023. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10993-023-09679-y.
Sevinç Y. Mindsets and family language pressure: language or anxiety transmission across generations? J Multiling Multicult Dev. 2022;43(9):874–90. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/01434632.2022.2038614.
Suggate S, Schaughency E, McAnally H, Reese E. From infancy to adolescence: The longitudinal links between. vocabulary, early literacy skills, oral narrative, and reading comprehension. Cognitive Development. 2018:47;82-95
Kraus MW, Piff PK, Mendoza-Denton R, Rheinschmidt ML, Keltner D. Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: how the rich are different from the poor. Psychol Rev. 2012;119(3):546–72. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/a0028756.
Levine D, Pace A, Luo R, Hirsh-Pasek K, Michnick Golinkoff R, de Villiers J, Iglesias A, Wilson S. Evaluating socioeconomic gaps in preschoolers’ vocabulary, syntax and language process skills with the quick interactive language screener (QUILS). Early Child Res Q. 2020;50:114–28. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.11.006.
Bonifacci P, Ferrara IC, Pedrinazzi J, Terracina F, Palladino P. Literacy acquisition trajectories in bilingual language minority children and monolingual peers with similar or different SES: a three-year longitudinal study. Brain Sci. 2022. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/brainsci12050563.
Bonifacci P, Lombardo G, Pedrinazzi J, Terracina F, Palladino P. Literacy skills in Bilinguals and monolinguals with different SES. Read Writ Q. 2020;36(3):243–59. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/10573569.2019.1635057.
Munir J, Faiza M, Jamal B, Daud S, Iqbal K. The impact of socio-economic status on academic achievement. J Soc Sci Rev. 2023;3(2):695–705. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.54183/jssr.v3i2.308.
Bergelson E, Soderstrom M, Schwarz IC, Rowland CF, Ramírez-Esparza N, R. Hamrick L, Cristia A. Everyday language input and production in 1001 children from six continents. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2023;120(52):e2300671120. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1073/pnas.2300671120.
Elliott L. Sources of heterogeneity in the home learning environments of socioeconomically disadvantaged families. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2020;70:101190. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.appdev.2020.101190.
Hemmerechts K, Agirdag O, Kavadias D. The relationship between parental literacy involvement, socio-economic status and reading literacy. Educ Rev. 2017;69(1):85–101. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/00131911.2016.1164667.
Gathercole VCM, Kennedy I, Thomas EM. Socioeconomic level and bilinguals’ performance on language and cognitive measures. Biling Lang Cogn. 2016;19(5):1057–78. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S1366728915000504.
Gatt D, Baldacchino R, Dodd B. Which measure of socioeconomic status best predicts bilingual lexical abilities and how? A focus on four-year-olds exposed to two majority languages. J Child Lang. 2020;47(4):737–65. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0305000919000886.
Chiat S, Polišenská K. A framework for crosslinguistic nonword repetition tests: effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on children’s performance. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2016;59(5):1179–89. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0293.
Meir N, Armon-Lotem S. Independent and combined effects of socioeconomic status (SES) and bilingualism on children’s vocabulary and verbal short-term memory. Front Psychol. 2017. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01442.
De Cat C. Predicting language proficiency in bilingual children. Stud Second Lang Acquis. 2020;42(2):279–325. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0272263119000597.
Kuo Y-L, Casillas A, Walton KE, Way JD, Moore JL. The intersectionality of race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status on social and emotional skills. J Res Pers. 2020;84:103905. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jrp.2019.103905.
Guhn M, Emerson SD, Mahdaviani D, Gadermann AM. Associations of birth factors and socio-economic status with indicators of early emotional development and mental health in childhood: a population-based linkage study. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2020;51(1):80–93. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-019-00912-6.
Reiss F. Socioeconomic inequalities and mental health problems in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Soc Sci Med. 2013;90:24–31. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.04.026.
Yoshikawa H, Aber JL, Beardslee WR. The effects of poverty on the mental, emotional, and behavioural health of children and youth: implications for prevention. Am Psycholgist. 2012;67(4):272–84. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/a0028015.
Peverill M, Dirks MA, Narvaja T, Herts KL, Comer JS, McLaughlin KA. Socioeconomic status and child psychopathology in the United States: a meta-analysis of population-based studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2021;83:101933. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101933.
Costello EJ, Keeler GP, Angold A. Poverty, race/ethnicity, and psychiatric disorder: a study of rural children. Am J Public Health. 2001;91(9):1494–8. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.2105/ajph.91.9.1494.
McLaughlin KA, Costello EJ, Leblanc W, Sampson NA, Kessler RC. Socioeconomic status and adolescent mental disorders. Am J Public Health. 2012;102(9):1742–50. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.2105/ajph.2011.300477.
Singh L, Rajendra SJ. Greater attention to socioeconomic status in developmental research can improve the external validity, generalizability, and replicability of developmental science. Dev Sci. 2024. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/desc.13521.
Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(11):1337–45. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015.
Runge RA, Soellner R. Measuring children’s emotional and behavioural problems: are SDQ parent reports from native and immigrant parents comparable? Child Adolesc Psychiatry Mental Health. 2019;13(1):46. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13034-019-0306-z.
George D, Mallery P. SPSS for Windows step by step: a simple guide and reference. 11.0 update—fourth edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 2003.
Li JB, Delvecchio E, Di Riso D, Lis A, Salcuni S. Early evidence of the Italian parent-report strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ-P). Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2017;48:335–45. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-016-0646-3.
Tobia V, Marzocchi GM. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire-parents for Italian school-aged children: psychometric properties and norms. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2018;49:1–8. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-017-0723-2.
Bonifacci P, Compiani D, Ravaldini V, Peri B, Affranti A, Tobia V. A multi-informant approach testing an expanded home numeracy mode. Education Sciences. 2023:13(3);278.
Bonifacci P, Compiani D, Vassura C, Affranti A, Peri B, Ravaldini V, Tobia. Home Learning Environment and Screen Time Differentially Mediate the Relationship Between Socioeconomic Status and Preschoolers’ Learning and Behavioural Profiles. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2024:1-14.
Burani C, Barca L, Arduino LS. Una base Di Dati Sui valori di età di acquisizione, frequenza, familiarità, immaginabilità, concretezza, e altre variabili lessicali e sublessicali per 626 nomi dell’italiano. Giornale Italiano Di Psicologia. 2001;4:839–56. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1421/379.
RStudio Team. (2020). RStudio: integrated development for R. In RStudio, PBC. http://www.rstudio.com/
Zeileis A, Hothorn T. Diagnostic checking in regression relationships. R News. 2002;2(3):7–10.
Fox J, Weisberg S. An R companion to applied regression. 3rd ed. Sage; 2019.
Harrell FJ. (2023). Hmisc: Harrell Miscellaneous. R package version 5.1-2. In https://hbiostat.org/R/Hmisc
Groemping U. Relative importance for linear regression in R: the package relaimpo. J Stat Softw. 2006;17(1):1–27. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.18637/jss.v017.i01.
Bonifacci P, Tobia V, Bernabini L, Marzocchi GM. Early literacy and numeracy skills in bilingual minority children: toward a relative independence of linguistic and numerical processing. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1020. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01020.
Bonifacci P, Barbieri M, Tomassini M, Roch M. In few words: linguistic gap but adequate narrative structure in preschool bilingual children. J Child Lang. 2018;45(1):120–47. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0305000917000149.
Liu G, Zhao Z, Li B, Pan Y, Cheng G. Parental psychological well-being and parental emotional warmth as mediators of the relationship between family socioeconomic status and children’s life satisfaction. Curr Psychol. 2023;42(27):23958–65. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s12144-022-03568-z.
Bonifacci P, Giombini L, Bellocchi S, Contento S. Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Dev Sci. 2011;14(2):256–69. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00974.x.
Poarch GJ, Krott A. A Bilingual advantage? An appeal for a change in perspective and recommendations for future research. Behav Sci. 2019;9(9):95.
Acknowledgements
We thank Chiara Vassura, Diego Compiani, Alexandra Affranti and Benedetta Peri for collaboration in data collection.
Funding
The was conducted within the collaboration agreement funded by Municipality of Bologna, “Convenzione per la definizione di attività di ricerca e progettazione volte alla definizione di un sistema di percorsi di qualificazione dell’offerta formativa delle scuole dell’infanzia e per la formazione di personale educativo e docente, A.A. 2021/2022” [Agreement for the definition of research and planning activities aimed at defining a system of qualification paths for the educational offer of kindergartens and for the training of educational and teaching staff, a.y. 2021/22].
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization, P.B. and V.T.; methodology, P.B., V.T.; formal analysis, M.G.; investigation, V.R.; resources, P.B.; data curation, P.B., V.T., M.G.; writing—original draft preparation, V.D., P.B., M.G.; writing—review and editing, P.B., V.T.; supervision, P.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethical approval
Participants involved in the study gave informed consent, the study was conducted in accordance in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the University of Bologna Bioethical Committee approved the project (Prot. 322431, December 21, 2021).
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Bonifacci, P., Ravaldini, V., Cangelosi, M. et al. Socioeconomic status, linguistic skills and language background differentially relate to preschoolers’ emotional and behavioural profile. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 18, 133 (2024). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13034-024-00823-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13034-024-00823-y